The power of music
- E-mail: mail@oliversacks.com
What an odd thing it is to see an entire
species—billions of people—playing with listening to meaningless tonal
patterns,
occupied and preoccupied for much of their time by
what they call ‘music.’ This, at least, was one of the things about
human
beings that puzzled the highly cerebral alien beings,
the Overlords, in Arthur C. Clarke's novel Childhood's End.
Curiosity brings them down to the Earth's surface to attend a concert;
they listen politely and patiently, and at the end,
congratulate the composer on his ‘great
ingenuity’—while still finding the entire business unintelligible. They
cannot think
what goes on in human beings when they make or listen
to music, because nothing goes on within them. They, themselves, as
a species, lack music.
Clarke likes to embody questions in fables,
and the Overlords' bewilderment makes one wonder, indeed, what it is
about music
that gives it such peculiar power over us, a power
delectable and beneficent for the most part, but also capable of
uncontrollable
and sometimes destructive force.
We may imagine the Overlords ruminating
further, back in their spaceships. This so-called ‘music,’ they would
have to concede,
is in some way efficacious to humans. Yet it has no
concepts, and makes no propositions; it lacks images, symbols, the stuff
of language. It has no power of representation. It has
no relation to the world. These, indeed, are the very issues
Schopenhauer
raises in The World as Will and Representation—and Schopenhauer himself was passionately musical. Music, for him, was an embodiment of pure ‘will’—but this is not a notion
that goes down well in a neuroscientific age.
Another passionately musical philosopher,
Nietzsche, said, ‘We listen to music with our muscles.’ This, at least,
is something
we can see. It is evident in all of us—we tap our
feet, we ‘keep time’, hum, sing along or ‘conduct’ music, our facial
expressions
mirroring the rises and falls, the melodic contours
and feelings of what we are hearing. Yet all this may occur without our
knowledge or volition.
All this is normal, and may be seen as a
half-conscious resonance to music, a sort of involuntary personal
expression as the
music works on us. But these effects, the overflow of
music into the motor system, can easily go too far, becoming
irresistible
and perhaps even coercive.
Anthony Storr, in his excellent book Music and the Mind,
stresses that in all societies, a primary function of music is
collective and communal, to bring and bind people together.
People sing together, dance together, in every
culture, and one can imagine them doing so, around the first fires, a
hundred
thousand years ago. This primal role of music is to
some extent lost today, when we have a special class of composers and
performers, and the rest of us are often reduced to
passive listening. One has to go to a concert, or a church or a musical
festival, to recapture the collective excitement and
bonding of music. In such a situation, there seems to be an actual
binding
of nervous systems, the unification of an audience by a
veritable ‘neurogamy’ (to use a word favoured by early Mesmerists).
It is easy to be overcome, for better or
worse, in a communal setting. One of the most dramatic effects of
music's power is
the induction of trance states, which have been
described by ethnomusicologists in nearly every culture. Trance—ecstatic
singing
and dancing, wild movements and cries, perhaps,
rhythmic rocking, or catatonia-like rigidity or immobility—involves both
motor
and gross emotional, psychic and autonomic effects,
culminating in profoundly altered states of consciousness; and whilst
it can be achieved by a single individual, it often
seems to be facilitated in a communal group. I have not encountered
music-induced
trance in the course of clinical practice, but it has
been documented by countless films, and experienced by many thousands
of people, whether in concerts, drum circles or
meditation—and it has been used by various religions for millennia.
Gilbert
Rouet, in his monumental book, Music and Trance, discusses these phenomena at length.
We see the coercive power of music if it is
of excessive volume, or has an overwhelming beat, at rock concerts where
thousands
of people, as one, may be taken over, engulfed or
entrained by the music, just as the beat of war drums can incite extreme
martial excitement and solidarity. (There is now,
indeed, a whole genre of modern dance music called ‘Trance,’ designed to
have such an effect.) Mickey Hart and others have
written eloquently of the power of drumming in cultures all over the
world,
and here it is especially the dynamic power of rhythm
that is pre-eminent.
This motor power of rhythm may be especially
strong in various forms of motor and impulse disorder—and music can
indeed be
therapeutic here. Thus, patients with parkinsonism, in
whom movements tend to be incontinently fast or slow or sometimes
frozen,
may overcome these disorders of timing when they are
exposed to the regular tempo and rhythm of music. The eminent (and now
parkinsonian) composer Lukas Foss, for example, whom I
saw recently, may festinate or rocket almost uncontrollably to his
piano, but once he is there, can play a Chopin
nocturne with exquisite control and timing and grace—only to festinate
or freeze
once more as soon as the music ends.
Music is profoundly important to those with
motor disorders, though the music must be of the ‘right’
kind—suggestive, but
not peremptory—or things may go wrong. For one of my
deeply parkinsonian post-encephalitic patients, Frances D., music was
as powerful as any drug. One minute I would see her
compressed, clenched and blocked, or else jerking, ticking and
jabbering—like
a sort of human time bomb. The next minute, if we
played music for her, all of these explosive–obstructive phenomena would
disappear, replaced by a blissful ease and flow of
movement, as Mrs D., suddenly freed of her automatisms, would smilingly
‘conduct’ the music, or rise and dance to it. But it
was necessary—for her—that the music be legato; for staccato, percussive
music might have a bizarre counter-effect, causing her
to jump and jerk helplessly with the beat, like a mechanical doll or
marionette.
People with Tourette's syndrome—including
many I know who are professional musicians—may become composed,
tic-free, when they
listen to or perform music; but they may also be
driven by certain kinds of music into an uncontrollable ticcing that is
entrained
with the beat.
The stirring or animating power of music
entails emotional no less than motor arousal. We turn to music, we need
it, because
of its ability to move us, to induce feelings and
moods, states of mind. Therapeutically, this power can be very striking
in people with autism or frontal lobe syndromes, who
may otherwise have little access to strong emotional states. And the
evocative power of music can also be of immense value
in people with Alzheimer's disease or other dementias, who may have
become unable to understand or respond to language,
but can still be profoundly moved—and often regain their cognitive
focus,
at least for a while—when exposed to music, especially
familiar music that may evoke for them memories of earlier events,
encounters or states of mind that cannot be called up
in any other way. Music may bring them back briefly to a time when the
world was much richer for them.
But music can also be profoundly evocative,
have deep resonances, without being familiar, and without calling up
specific
memories. All of us have had the experience of being
transported by the sheer beauty of music—suddenly finding ourselves in
tears, not knowing whether they are of joy or sadness,
suddenly feeling a sense of the sublime, or a great stillness within.
I do not know how to characterize these transcendent
emotions, but they can still be evoked, as far as I can judge, even in
deeply demented (and sometimes agitated or tormented)
patients. Music can bring them, if only for a little while, a sense
of clarity, joy and tranquility.
Although we are now becoming attuned to the
many forms of amusia that may result from cortical damage, and might
expect that
people with extensive cortical damage would lose the
ability to recognize or respond to music, one finds instead that even
people with advanced Alzheimer's disease can
nonetheless respond to the evocative powers of music. Isabelle Peretz
and others
have called attention to this, and the need for
investigating how such responsiveness is maintained.
There is a wide range of sensitivity to the
emotional power of music, ranging from virtual indifference at one
extreme (Freud
was said to be indifferent to music, and never wrote
about it), to a sensitivity that can barely be controlled. Individuals
with Williams syndrome, for example, though they have
severe visual and cognitive defects, are often musically gifted, and
usually extravagantly sensitive to the emotional
impact of music. I have seen few sights more extraordinary than a group
of
40 young people with Williams syndrome breaking into
uncontrollable weeping at tender or sad music, or uncontrollably excited
if the music is animated.
That music and especially melody can be
profoundly evocative is clear. But what is it that is evoked? Anthony
Storr quotes
Suzanne Langer and Marcel Proust in this regard. For
Proust, ‘Music … helped me to descend into myself, to discover new
things;
the variety that I had sought in vain in life, in
travel, but a longing for which was nonetheless renewed in me by this
sonorous
tide.’ Jonathan Miller, the opera director, summarizes
this in a single word: the real role of music, in his view, is not
to take one outside oneself, but to take one inside
oneself. Suzanne Langer goes further, writing that music not only has
the power to recall past emotions, but to evoke
‘emotions and moods we have not felt, passions we did not know before.’
This wonderful, enlarging power, can,
however, be deeply upsetting to those who need to have their emotions
and imaginations
under tight control—whether this be obsessive or
artistic. Thus, Tolstoy was deeply ambivalent about music—it had, he
felt,
a power to induce in him ‘fictitious’ states of mind,
emotions and images that were not his own, and not under his control.
He adored Tschaikovsky's music, but often refused to
listen to it, and in The Kreutzer Sonata, he described the
seduction of the narrator's wife by a violinist and his music—the two of
them play Beethoven's Kreutzer
Sonata together, and this music is so powerful, the
narrator comes to think, that it can change a woman's heart, and cause
her to be unfaithful. The story ends with the outraged
husband murdering his wife—though the real enemy, he feels, the enemy
he cannot kill, is the music.
Indeed, for many of us the emotions induced
by music may be overwhelming—Robert Zatorre speaks of ‘chills’ in this
connection,
and has shown some of the specific neurobiological
basis of these. An eminent psychologist friend of mine, who is intensely
sensitive to music, cannot have it on as background
when he works; he must attend to music completely, or turn it off, for
it is too powerful to allow him to focus on any other
mental activity. States of ecstasy and rapture may lie in wait for us,
if we give ourselves totally to music, and these, of
course, also carry the danger of excess. A common scene during the 1950s
was to see entire audiences swooning in response to
Frank Sinatra or Elvis Presley—seized by an emotional and perhaps erotic
excitement so intense as to induce fainting. Richard
Wagner, too, was a master of the musical manipulation of emotions, and
this, perhaps, is a reason why his music is so
intoxicating to some, and so avoided by others.
The motor and emotional effects of music are
visible, largely, whereas the perception or the imagining of music is
internal,
and we are dependent here on reports from listeners
(though now we are beginning to visualize the neural basis of their
experiences
by functional brain imaging). When Francis Galton
wrote on ‘mental imagery’ in the 1880s, he concerned himself only with
visual
imagery, and not at all with musical imagery. But a
tally of one's friends will suffice to show that musical imagery has a
range no less varied than the visual. There are some
people who can scarcely hold a tune in their heads, and others who can
hear entire symphonies in their minds, with a detail
and vividness little short of actual perception.
There are some singular attributes of
musical imagery (and musical memory) that have no equivalents in the
visual sphere,
and these may cast light on the fundamentally
different way in which the brain treats music. This peculiarity of music
may
be in part because we have to construct a visual world
for ourselves, and a selective and personal character therefore infuses
our visual memories from the start—whereas we are
given pieces of music already constructed, musical objects that will be
retained, if they are retained, with an almost
phonographic fidelity. A visual or social scene can be represented in a
hundred
different ways, but the recall of a musical piece has
to be close to the original. We do, of course, listen selectively, with
differing interpretations and emotions, but the basic
musical characteristics of a piece—its tempo, its rhythm, its melodic
contours, even its pitch—tend to be preserved with
remarkable fidelity. It is this fidelity—this almost defenceless
engraving
of music on the brain—which may play a crucial part in
predisposing us to certain excesses, or pathologies, of musical imagery
and memory, excesses that may even occur in relatively
‘unmusical’ people.
All of us have experienced the involuntary,
helpless mental replaying of songs or tunes, or snatches of music we
have just
been exposed to, by chance, even, perhaps, without
‘listening’ consciously. We call such tunes ‘catchy’—and they are
sometimes
referred to as ‘earworms,’ for they may burrow into
us, entrench themselves and then perseverate internally hundreds of
times
a day, only to evaporate, fade away, in a day or two,
perhaps to be followed by the next earworm. This often meaningless
perseveration
is quite unlike voluntary musical imagery, and unlike
the involuntary musical imagery that may be evoked by a sight, a sound,
a word, with some significant, though often
unconscious, association (although this may then turn into a earworm).
Perseverative
music has much more the character of a cerebral
automatism, suggesting cerebral networks, perhaps both cortical and
subcortical,
caught in a circuit of mutual excitation. I do not
think there are comparable phenomena with other types of
perception—certainly
not with visual experience. For instance, I am a
verbal creature myself, and though sentences often permute themselves in
my mind and suddenly surface as I am writing, I never
have verbal ‘earworms’ comparable with musical ones.
Unlike these earworms, true musical
hallucinations are experienced by those who have them as unprecedented
and deeply disquieting.
There is insufficient awareness among physicians of
musical hallucinations, in part because patients are reluctant to report
them, fearing that they will be dismissed or seen as
‘crazy’. But musical hallucinations are surprisingly common, affecting
at least 2% of those who are losing their hearing, as
well as patients with a variety of other conditions. Working with a
population of elderly patients (though I have seen it
in younger people as well), I am often given vivid descriptions of
musical
hallucinosis, and I think it is by far the most common
form of non-psychotic hallucination. I related two stories of musical
hallucination in my 1985 book The Man Who Mistook his Wife for a Hat,
and since then have received hundreds of letters from people with this
condition. With musical hallucinations it is common
for several voices or instruments to be heard
simultaneously, and such experiences are almost always attributed,
initially,
to an external source. Thus in 1995 I received a vivid
letter from June M., a charming and creative woman of 70, telling me
of her musical hallucinations:
‘most of the music I hear is from my past—many of the songs are hymns, some are folk music, some pop up from the forties and fifties, some classical and some show tunes. All the selections are sung by a chorus—there is never a solo performance or any orchestration. This first started last November when I was visiting my sister and brother in law in Cape Hatteras, NC, one night. After turning off the TV and preparing to retire, I started hearing ‘Amazing Grace.’ It was being sung by a choir, over and over again. I checked with my sister to see if they had some church service on TV, but they had Monday night football, or some such. So I went onto the deck overlooking Pamlico Sound. The music followed me. I looked down on the quiet coastline and the few houses with lights and realized that the music couldn't possibly be coming from anywhere in that area. It had to be in my head’.
It was not clear why June M. started to have
musical hallucinations, or why she still has them, 11 years later. She
has excellent
hearing, is not epileptic, has no known medical
problems and is intellectually quite intact. With her, as with many
other
patients, the most searching examination may fail to
pinpoint the cause of musical hallucinations; though if she had a PET
scan, it would, I suspect, show the widespread
activation of cortical and subcortical networks that T. D. Griffiths has
found
in several subjects with musical hallucinations—an
activation very similar to what occurs when one is actually listening to
music.
Musical hallucinations sometimes emerge
insidiously, bit by bit, but more commonly come on suddenly and
full-blown, and without
obvious ‘cause.’ They most commonly ‘replay’ music
heard in childhood, but perhaps not consciously recalled for decades.
Musical
hallucinations tend to be highly repetitive, sometimes
with a single theme or sequence of notes being repeated again and
again.
One of my patients, tiring of an endless megaphonic
rendering of ‘O Come, All Ye Faithful’, tried to block this by playing
a Chopin étude, only to find that four bars of the
Chopin then iterated themselves in her mind non-stop for the next 24 h.
This sort of experience is common among those
afflicted with musical hallucinations—not only regurgitation of early
musical
experiences but also a tendency to instant ‘replay’ of
music just heard—a sort of palimusia. Musical hallucinations cannot
be stopped by an effort of will, though they can
sometimes be changed, especially to music with a similar rhythmic or
melodic
character. Another correspondent wrote in this regard
of his ‘intracranial jukebox.’ Musical hallucinations can be very loud
and interfere with perception or conversation in a
manner that never occurs with normal musical imagery.
The term ‘release hallucination’ was coined
in the early 1970s for the occurrence of visual or auditory
hallucinations associated
with impaired sight or hearing. But the concept of
‘release’ goes back a century or more to Hughlings Jackson's notions of
neural functions or phenomena being held in check by
inhibition, and ‘released,’ therefore, if inhibition is sufficiently
diminished. There are rich reciprocal connections
between our sense organs and the brain—essential for understanding the
central
modulation of perception—and it is postulated that
normal sensory activity serves to inhibit too much retrograde activity
in these. But if this inhibition is critically
diminished, through sensory impairment or lack of stimulus, then a sort
of
reflux may occur in the form of hallucinations. (One
might suppose that restoring hearing with hearing aids or cochlear
implants
would stop this reflux, but it rarely does; one
profoundly deaf patient of mine with musical hallucinations was given a
cochlear
implant, and while this has given her a whole new
auditory world, it has done little to change her musical
hallucinations.)
Yet deafness almost never leads to hallucinations of
voices, only to hallucinations of music (voices may be heard in the
words
of a lyric, but not muttering or talking). That
musical hallucinations thus take precedence over all other auditory
hallucinations
shows again the special and potent character of the
neural processing of music.
Although musicogenic epilepsy has probably
existed as long as epilepsy itself, the condition was only formally
recognized
and named by Macdonald Critchley in the early 1930s
(he himself preferred the more euphonious term ‘musicolepsia’).
Musicogenic
epilepsy is generally considered to be very rare, but
Critchley wondered if it might be notably more common than supposed.
For many people, he thought, might start to get a
queer feeling—disturbing, perhaps frightening—when they heard certain
music,
but then would immediately retreat from the music,
turn it off, or block their ears, so that they did not progress to a
full-blown
seizure. He wondered, therefore, if ‘formes frustes’
of musical epilepsy might be relatively common. (This has certainly been
my own impression, and I think there may also be
similar formes frustes of photic epilepsy, when blinking lights or
fluorescent
lights may produce a peculiar discomfort without
producing a full-blown seizure.)
Working in an epilepsy clinic, I have seen a
number of patients with seizures induced by music, and others who have
musical
auras associated with seizures—occasionally both. Both
types of patient have temporal lobe seizures, and, in most cases,
temporal
lobe lesions identifiable with brain imaging. Among
the patients I have seen recently is G.G., a young man who was in good
health until June 2005, when he had a severe attack of
herpes encephalitis that started with a high fever and generalized
seizures, followed by coma, and severe amnesia.
Remarkably, a year later, his amnestic problems have virtually cleared
(though
his encephalitis had involved both temporal lobes),
but he remains highly seizure-prone, with occasional grand mal seizures
and, much more commonly, complex partial seizures.
Initially, all of these were ‘spontaneous,’ but within a few weeks they
started to occur almost exclusively in response to
sound—‘sudden, loud sounds, like ambulance sirens’—and, especially,
music.
Along with this G.G. developed hyperacusis, becoming
able to detect sounds too soft or distant for others to hear. He enjoyed
this, and felt that his auditory world was now ‘more
alive, more vivid,’ but wondered, too, whether it played any part in
his now epileptic sensitivity to music and sound.
Unlike another patient, Mrs N., who has
seizures only in response to Neapolitan songs, G.G.'s attacks may be
provoked by a
large range of music, from rock to classical (the
first time I saw him, he played a Verdi aria on his cell phone, and
after
about half a minute this induced a complex partial
seizure). He speaks of ‘romantic’ music as being the most provocative,
especially Frank Sinatra's songs (‘He touches a chord
in me’). He says that the music has to be ‘full of emotions,
associations,
nostalgia’—it is almost always music he has known from
childhood or adolescence. It does not have to be loud to provoke a
seizure; soft music may be equally effective. His
seizures start with, or are preceded by, a special state of intense,
involuntary,
almost forced, attention or listening. In this already
altered state, the music seems to grow more intense, to swell, to take
possession of him, and at this point, he cannot stop
the process, cannot turn off the music or walk away from it. Beyond this
point he retains no consciousness or memory, although
various ictal automatisms and automatic behaviours ensue. For Mr G.,
music does not just provoke a seizure, it seems to
constitute an essential part of the seizure, spreading (one imagines)
from
its initial perceptual locus to other temporal lobe
systems, as these are activated in temporal lobe seizures and
occasionally
reaching the motor cortex, when he has generalized
seizures.
I have sometimes been given similar
descriptions by patients whose seizures are not provoked by music, but
contain hallucinatory
music as a prominent feature. (Perhaps this is not
surprising, for, as Penfield remarked, ‘The localization for production
of [epileptic] music is in the superior temporal
convolution … and, as such, close to the point associated with so-called
musicogenic epilepsy’.) One such patient, Eric M., who
has an astrocytoma and temporal lobe seizures, hears music during his
seizures that he cannot identify, even though he is a
musician, but he finds this music hauntingly familiar. ‘Once I become
aware of that strange yet familiar confusion,’ he
says, ‘and realize it is in fact a seizure, I seem to try not to figure
out what the music could be … . I am afraid that if I
pay too much attention to it, I may not be able to escape it—like
quicksand,
or hypnosis.’ He may, nonetheless, be drawn deeper and
deeper into it, until he realizes that it is out of his hands.
Our auditory systems, our nervous systems,
are tuned for music. Perhaps we are a musical species no less than a
linguistic
one. But there seems to be in us a peculiar
sensitivity to music, a sensitivity that can all too easily slip out of
control,
become excessive, become a susceptibility or a
vulnerability. Too-muchness lies continually in wait, whether this takes
the
form of ‘earworms’, musical hallucinations, swoons and
trances, or music-induced seizures. This is the other side of the
otherwise
wonderful power of music. How much this is due to the
intrinsic characteristics of music itself—its complex sonic patterns
woven in time, its logic, its momentum, its
unbreakable sequences, its insistent rhythms and repetitions, the
mysterious way
in which it embodies emotion and ‘will’—and how much
to special resonances, synchronizations, oscillations, mutual
excitations,
feedbacks, and so forth, in the immensely complex,
multi-level neural circuitry that subserves musical perception and
replay,
we do not know. We do not even know why, for instance,
simple stroboscopic light displays can excite hallucinations, myoclonus
and seizures, and this is an infinitely simpler matter
than the powers of music.
When Crichtley and Henson's Music and the Brain
was published in 1977, functional brain imaging still lay in the
future, and neuroscience had yet to approach the neural
correlates of musical perception, imagery and memory
or their disorders. In the last 20 years, there have been huge advances
here, but we have, as yet, scarcely touched the
question of why music, for better or worse, has so much power. It is a
question
that goes to the heart of being human.
- © The Author (2006). Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Guarantors of Brain. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar